The Absurdity Of Asset Allocation Studies

Updated on

Determinants of Portfolio Performance,” the seminal 1986 paper on asset allocation by Gary P. Brinson, Randolph Hood, and Gilbert L. Beebower (BHB), begins by citing a study showing that most corporate pension funds focus their attention solely on the problem of manager selection. BHB’s research presents evidence, however, that 93.6% of total return variation is due not to manager selection, but to asset allocation. Often, the takeaway is that the variation in performance across investment funds is 93.6% attributable to asset allocation, and only marginally to manager decisions such as security selection and market timing.

Get The Full Warren Buffett Series in PDF

Get the entire 10-part series on Warren Buffett in PDF. Save it to your desktop, read it on your tablet, or email to your colleagues

Also read:

  • Baupost Letter Points To Concern Over Risk Parity, Systematic Strategies During Crisis
  • AI Hedge Fund Robots Beating Their Human Masters

But that’s not what it says at all.

The race is to the swift - or is it?

Let’s suppose you are doing research on the performance of 100 racers who run the Boston marathon – a hilly course over which lap times change from mile to mile. The runners all do their own stretching exercises before running, and they drink the energy drink and eat the granola bar that are handed out by the race sponsors.

You want to know how much the energy drink and the granola bar contribute to their performance. So here is what you do.

You have each runner run the course four times. The first time it is run, each runner does their stretching exercises and drinks the energy drink and eats the granola bar, and you record each runner’s times over the miles of the course and call them data series A.

The second time the runner stretches and only drinks the energy drink, and the recorded times are data series B. The third time the runner stretches and only eats the granola bar and the times are data series C, and the fourth time the runner only stretches and the times are data series D.

Then following BHB, you compute the correlation coefficient of D against A, C against A, and B against A (BHB call them “regressions”), and square each correlation coefficient to get an R-squared. The results are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. R-squareds of correlations of 26-mile sequences of running times

Only energy drink (sequence D), against both energy drink and granola bar (sequence A) 97.8%
Only granola bar (C ), against both (A) 95.3%
Only stretching (B), against both (A) 93.6%

The conclusion is that 93.6% of the variation in running times is explained by stretching, and only a relatively tiny percentage by the energy drink and the granola bar.

Huh?

What does the variation over the miles of the course have to do with this?

You thought we were going to do something to measure how the runners’ times for completing the marathon varied depending on whether they drank the energy drink, ate the granola bar, or only stretched. But it seems we did something different.

For some reason we focused on the variations within the sequences of running times over the 26 miles of the course, not the total times. And for some reason we computed the correlations between those sequences.

Of course, all of the sequences are going to have high correlations to each other. The variations are because some miles are uphill and some miles are downhill, not because of whether you drank the energy drink or ate the energy bar or stretched. You could have picked any of the combinations and it would “explain” more than 90% of the variation. Every time you run the course your mile-by-mile times are going to correlate closely, no matter what else you did.

Is there any reason to conclude from these results that the most important thing to do is to stretch – and that it is much more important than whether you have an energy drink or a granola bar? Of course not.

By Michael Edesess, read the full article here.

Leave a Comment